Schlagwort-Archive: 2017

Von der Blümchenwiese zur Monokultur?

Hamburg,  5. Juli 2017

Soziale Medien in 25 Kategorien – fast alle sind auch für die Wissenschaft und Wissenschaftler relevant. Quelle: Ethority, Version 6

Die deutschen Akademien der Wissenschaften legen sich mit Facebook, Twitter und anderen sozialen Netzwerken an. Sie müssten „verstärkt unter publizistischen/medien-rechtlichen Gesichtspunkten reguliert werden und nicht, wie es bisher überwiegend der Fall ist, primär unter ökonomischen und kartellrechtlichen Aspekten“, fordern die Forscher. Eine 15-köpfige Arbeitsgruppe von Leopoldina, acatech und der Union der deutschen Akademien der Wissenschaften hat in Berlin die Stellungnahme „Social Media und digitale Wissenschaftskommunikation“ vorgestellt und mit Experten diskutiert.

Die Gruppe sehe „die freie Informationsverbreitung gefährdet“, sagte Holger Wormer von der Universität Dortmund, einer der Sprecher: „Wer anderen als Medium eine erhebliche Reichweite zur Verfügung stellt, muss für ein Minimum an Redaktion sorgen“ – dazu seien zum Beispiel auch Tageszeitungen auf ihren Leserbriefseiten verpflichtet.

Dass sozialen Medien gefälschte Meldungen, populistische Parolen und gezielte Diffamierungen verbreiten, ist kein originäres Problem der Wissenschaft. Den Verheißungen der Netzwerke auf bessere, schnellere und direkte Kommunikation folgte stets Enttäuschung, zeigt die Akademien-Arbeitsgruppe für die Gesellschaft als Ganze und für den eigenen Bereich: Freiheit, Gleichheit, Vielfalt und Transparenz zum Beispiel hätten sich eben nicht verbessert. Im öffentlichen Diskurs sei eine „Verflachung und Verrohung“ zu bemerken, die Güte der verbreiteten Information nehme ab: „Es entfällt oft die Sicherung der Qualität“, heißt es in dem 72-seitigen Papier.

„Ein Grundton, als ob wir noch eine Ausstiegsoption hätten“

Das wird gerade für die Wissenschaft zum Problem, wenn sich etwa Falschmeldungen über Klimawandel oder Impfungen im Netz verbreiten. Der Staat müsse daher mit Auflagen für die Konzerne „sowohl für die Garantie der allgemeinen Informationsversorgung als auch speziell für die Versorgung der Öffentlichkeit mit Informationen über die Wissenschaft“ sorgen, heißt es in der Stellungnahme.

Dem stimmte im Prinzip auch Staatssekretär Gerd Billen vom Bundesjustizministerium zu: „Wir haben nicht unbedingt den Anspruch, dass privatwirtschaftliche Unternehmen die Demokratie fördern, aber schon, dass sie ihr nicht schaden.“ Etliche andere Politiker, etwa CDU-Fraktionschef Volker Kauder und Bundestagspräsident Norbert Lammert, hatten bereits gefordert, soziale Medien stärker zu regulieren. Das zügige Löschen rechtswidriger Inhalte als ersten Schritt soll nun das Netzwerkdurchsetzungsgesetz erzwingen, das der Bundestag in seiner letzten Sitzung vor der Sommerpause beschlossen hat.

In der deutschsprachigen Wissenschaftskommunikation herrschten aber noch keine Zustände wie etwa in der Auseinandersetzung etwa über die Flüchtlingspolitik, sagte der Social-Media-Manager der Helmholtz-Gemeinschaft, Henning Krause. „Dagegen ist das wie eine Blümchenwiese.“ Und er kritisierte an der Stellungnahme den „Grundton, als ob wir noch eine Ausstiegsoption hätten“. Wie andere Experten warb er bei der Präsentation in Berlin dafür, neben den Risiken auch die Chancen der sozialen Medien zu sehen. Auch etliche Kommentare im Netz (eine gute Übersicht gibt es bei Marcus Anhäuser) bescheinigen der Stellungnahme, ihr Thema eher verfehlt zu haben. „Bei den zwei wichtigsten Empfehlungen an die Wissenschaft eineinhalb vertane Chancen“, schrieb Reiner Korbmann auf seinem Blog „Wissenschaft kommuniziert„.

Das Papier der Akademien enthält insgesamt zwölf Empfehlungen. Etliche betreffen die Nutzung sozialer Medien durch Wissenschaftsorganisationen und Forscher selbst. So sollen zum Beispiel keine falschen Anreize gesetzt werden, so dass Wissenschaftler und Pressestellen von Instituten ihren Erfolg nach Klicks und Likes messen. Ein Schwerpunkt ist aber auch die Förderung des Journalismus, speziell des Wissenschaftsjournalismus. Er wirke, so die Stellungnahme, bisher als „neutraler Beobachter, der es dem Rezipienten zuallererst erlaubt, sich aufgrund einer möglichst neutralen Darstellung ein eigenständiges Urteil zu bilden“.

Eine zentrale Institution für die Wahrheit?

Viele Medienunternehmen stecken aber in der Krise, weil ihre Einnahmequellen auch wegen der Kostenlos-Praktiken im Internet wegbrechen. Hier regt die Arbeitsgruppe an, über eine zentrale „redaktionell unabhängige bundesweite Wissenschaftskommunikations- und Informationsplattform“ nachzudenken. Sie solle Berichte über Forschungsergebnisse „aggregieren, redaktionell bewerten und hinsichtlich ihrer Urheberschaft transparent machen“, aber frei vom Einfluss des Staates und der Wissenschaftsorganisationen arbeiten können.

Eine solche „Deutschland-Redaktion stieß bei etlichen Experten auf Widerspruch. Solle das etwa eine „zentrale Institution der Wahrheit“ werden, fragte Markus Weißkopf von der Organisation „Wissenschaft im Dialog“ polemisch. Eigentlich sei das Ideal doch pluralistische Berichterstattung. Franziska Badenschier vom „Science Media Center“ in Köln mahnte, man solle doch lieber die noch arbeitenden Journalisten stärken als eine solche Institution zu schaffen. Und die freie Journalistin Tanja Krämer, Mitbegründerin von Riff-Reporter, warnte: „Da verschwimmen die Grenzen von unabhängigem Journalismus und Wissenschaftskommunikation.“ Letzteres ist für Journalisten ein Synonym für interessengeleitete Wissenschafts-PR. Die Akademien-Arbeitsgruppe sieht darin freilich einen neutralen Oberbegriff. Holger Wormer erwiderte darum: „Das Ziel, die Gesellschaft mit zuverlässigen Informationen zu versorgen, ist gefährdet. Darum ist es fast schon egal, mit welcher Koalition von Wissenschaftskommunikation und Journalisten man das macht.“

Insgesamt könne die Wissenschaft durchaus mutiger in ihren Ideen und Forderungen sein, fasste am Ende Gerd Billen zusammen. Er präsentierte einige Einfälle, wobei er betonte, das sei nicht die Meinung der Regierung, sondern seine private. Wie in der Umweltpolitik müsse man „Kosten internalisieren“. Dieses Stichwort wird zum Beispiel verwendet, wenn die Betreiber von Kohlekraftwerken für das bisher kostenfreie Entsorgen von Kohlendioxid eine Abgabe bezahlen müssen. Wer „geistige Umweltverschmutzung“ betreibe, den könne man zum Beispiel verpflichten, auf eigene Rechnung professionelle journalistische Inhalte in seinem Netzwerk zu verbreiten. Und analog zur Initiative etlicher Europa-Politiker, jedem 18-jährigen ein Interrail-Ticket für eine Reise über den Kontinent zu schenken, schlug Billen vor, jungen Leuten Zeitungsabonnements zu bezahlen. Das würde gleich mehrere Fliegen mit einer Klappe schlagen: Die Medienkompetenz der jungen Leser stärken und die Journalisten fördern.

Hinweis: Dieser Text ist zuerst auf der Plattform Riff-Reporter erschienen.

Der March for Science und das Mittelalter

Hamburg, 24. April 2017

Science March in Hamburg: Nach der ersten Kundgebung geht es vom Rathausmarkt los
in Richtung Jungfernstieg. Foto: C. Schrader 

Am Samstag waren etwa 1500 bis 2000 Menschen auf dem Hamburger Rathausmarkt beim hiesigen March for Science. Es war einer von 600 auf dem ganzen Planeten, die in Solidarität mit dem Protest der amerikanischen Wissenschaftler stattfanden. Diese marschierten in Washington, andere demonstrierten auch in der Antarktis, unter Wasser vor Hawaii und in etwa 20 deutschen Städten, darunter Heidelberg, Berlin und München.

Als ich vor dem Hamburger Rathaus stand und dort den Reden zuhörte, dachte ich an eine Passage aus dem Buch „Eine kurze Geschichte der Menschheit“ von Yuval Noah Harari. Er definiert die Entwicklung unserer Spezies als eine Abfolge von drei Revolutionen, der kognitiven vor 70000 Jahren, der landwirtschaftlichen vor 12000 Jahren und der wissenschaftlichen vor 500 Jahren. In dieser sind wir noch mitten drin, und der Anlass der Demos war, dass wir gerade eine Art Konterrevolution erleben: Der Einfluss der Wissenschaft auf das Denken und die Politik soll zurückgedrängt werden. Um dem entgegenzutreten, forderten die Veranstalter der Märsche – und damit die Teilnehmer – eine Art Evidenz-basierter Politik, also gesellschaftliche Entscheidungen, die sich auf wissenschaftliche Erkenntnisse stützen, zumindest dort, wo sie vorhanden sind.

Foto von twitter, Bearbeitung: cs

Es ist interessant, wie Harari das Wesen der wissenschaftlichen Revolution definiert. Sie habe mit dem Eingeständnis des Unwissens begonnen, sagt er. Im Mittelalter war die Menschheit, jedenfalls in Europa, aber auch in anderen Kulturkreisen, der Meinung, alles Wissenswerte sei schon bekannt. Wer sich bildete, nahm dieses vorhandene Wissen in sich auf, niemand sah die Notwendigkeit, darüber hinaus zu denken. Was nicht bekannt war, war unwichtig. Und Empirie, die dem scheinbaren Wissen widersprach, konnte daran auch nichts ändern, manchmal war sie sogar gefährlich.

Dieses Bewusstsein änderte sich zu Beginn der Neuzeit. Harari illustriert das am Beispiel der „Entdeckung“ Amerikas. Christopher Columbus segelte 1492 in der festen Erwartung los, den Seeweg nach Indien zu finden. Er betrachtete die Karibik-Inseln, auf denen er landete, als Teil Indiens (noch heute heißen sie westindische Inseln) und nannte ihre Bewohner Indios. Bis zu seinem Tod beharrte er auf dieser Postion. Und dann kam der „erste moderne Mensch“ Amerigo Vespucci, der um das Jahr 1500 nach Europa von einem unbekannten Kontinent berichtete, vor dessen Küste Columbus‘ Inseln lägen. Es war ein Kontinent, von dem die Gelehrten seiner Zeit nichts wussten, der in ihren Büchern nicht vorkam, den es nach mittelalterlichem Denken nicht geben konnte. Harari urteilt: „Im Grunde ist es nur gerecht, dass ein Viertel der Welt nach einem unbekannten Italiener benannt wurde, an den wir uns heute nur deshalb erinnern, weil er den Mut hatte zu sagen: ,Wir wissen es nicht.'“

Das Weltbild des Mittelalters war natürlich stark geprägt von heiligen Büchern. Und die Menschen lebten in Ehrfurcht davor. „Es war unvorstellbar“, schreibt Harari, „dass die Bibel, der Koran oder die Vedas ein entscheidendes Geheimnis des Universums übersehen haben könnten, und dass es an gewöhnlichen Sterblichen sein könnte, dieses Geheimnis zu lüften… Was die mächtigen Götter nicht offenbarten und was die Weisen der Vergangenheit nicht in ihre Schriften aufnahmen, war definitionsgemäß irrelevant.“

Und da – jetzt spinne ich Hararis Analyse weiter –  haben wir doch eine Erklärung für die Wissenschafts-Feindlichkeit, die uns zum Beispiel aus den USA entgegenschlägt. Die religiöse Rechte, die dort jetzt den Ton angibt, beharrt darauf, dass Gott die Geschicke der Welt bis ins Detail lenkt, und verlangt Unterordnung unter die himmlische Fügung (und ganz nebenbei kann man dann noch die eigenen Interessen der biblischen Botschaft unterjubeln): Am klarsten zeigt sich das bei der Opposition gegen die Evolutionsforschung und den Evolutionsunterricht in den Schulen. Es ist aber auch in den Debatten über den Klimawandel, Impfungen oder Gentechnik zu erkennen. In dieser Hinsicht führt der Gegenwind gegen die Wissenschaften sozusagen in das Mittelalter zurück nach dem Motto: Es ist nicht am Menschen, die Geheimnisse Gottes zu lüften und zu entweihen.

Es ist nötig, sich diese Positionen einmal auf diese Weise klar zu machen. Natürlich will von den Anhängern der Trump-Regierung niemand zurück ins Mittelalter. Aber die geistige Haltung, der Wissenschaft keinen zentralen Wert mehr in der Gesellschaft einzuräumen, hat viele Parallelen zur damals verbreiteten Position des Nicht-Wissen-Wollens.

PS: Zwei Bemerkungen noch. Erstens: Dass es der Wissenschaft erfolgreich gelingt, viele Geheimnisse zu lüften, nehmen manche als Beleg, dass es Gott gar nicht gibt. Das halte ich für eine unzulässige Verkürzung, außerdem tritt man damit den Menschen vor das Schienenbein, für deren Identität der Glauben an Gott zentral ist. Das ist nicht nur grundlos verletzend, sondern auch sinnlos und taktisch unklug, weil es Reaktionen wie die jetzige beschleunigen kann. Zudem gibt es in den Kirchen viele kluge Menschen, die zwischen Religion und Wissenschaft überhaupt keinen Widerspruch sehen, auch und gerade auf den Gebieten Evolution und Klimawandel.
Zweitens: Dass sich Politik auf wissenschaftliche Erkenntnisse stützt, unterstütze ich natürlich vollkommen. Doch das greift zu kurz, denn die Resultate der Wissenschaft können gesellschaftliche Entscheidungen ja nicht ersetzen. In der Klimadebatte zum Beispiel kann man sehr wohl verschiedene Kurse steuern, wenn sie alle die Leitplanken einhalten, die die Wissenschaft gesteckt hat. Insofern müsste man eigentlich fordern, dass Politik den Erkenntnissen der Forschung nicht widersprechen sollte, sie darüber hinaus aber ihren Gestaltungsfreiraum ausschöpfen kann – und dass sich jeder in einer Demokratie daran beteiligen kann. Das ist vielleicht nicht sexy genug für den Aufruf zu einer Demonstration, aber zentral für das Selbstverständnis des Wissenschaftlers als Bürger. Wie heißt es beim IPCC: policy relevant but not policy prescriptive.

Scientific method and so-called skeptics

This is an addendum to the answer I have tried to give to Scott Adams‘ (the creator of Dilbert)
question on why science can’t seem to persuade climate skeptics. The basic answer is: Because they don’t want to be convinced. Maybe I should leave it at that, but I feel that maybe some people could indeed benefit by discussing the finer points of the methods of science Adams is targeting with his post. So this is for those specialists (this post not being announced anywhere but at the end of the main answer I have given).

Dear Scott Adams, I’ll dip into some of your points 1 through 14 here. I’ll go at my own pace, I am not necessarily going to jump the hoops you hold up. If you want to skip ahead: I will be dealing with

For starters this business about supplying a number, a percentage of how much of global warming was man-made. The IPCC said in 2013: “It is extremely likely that human influence has been the dominant cause of the observed warming since the mid-20th century.“ The UN-body elsewhere defines “extremely likely” as 95 to 100 percent probability. But it doesn’t give a number for “dominant”.

Let’s figure this out. Since the only other category except human influence would be non-human or natural influence we have a two-way-split. I think it would be safe then to assume that dominant means decidedly more than 50 percent. And if you look at this figure from that same IPCC report, you would think it is closer to 100 percent, wouldn’t you? Without human influence there would be little to no warming, that’s those blue bands. Add human and natural together and you get the pinkish bands around the actual observations. It’s clearer in larger regions with more data: temperatures in North America, Europe, Asia, Australia show no overlap of the colored bands anymore. And it’s most clear in global averages. And there are literally 1000s of sources in the IPCC report.

Source: IPCC, AR5, WG1, Figure SPM.6 

Maybe well over 50 to 100 percent human influence is still too vague for you. Sorry, things are not as simple as we all would like them to be. Anyways: Any single number would most certainly be evidently wrong in the precision it radiates. And attack would be sure to follow. Another of the mock debates surrounding this issue.

Next: Why do you fight being told the rate of warming was a tell-tale sign of what is happening? Rates are often as important or even more so than absolute numbers in every day life, too. Consider the task of sending a message from New York to San Francisco: The distance has stayed the same since both cities are on the map. First you could send it via stage coach, then by train, plane, now e-mail. The amount of time went from weeks to seconds and that means the speed of transmission, the rate of transport went up by orders of magnitude. Not such a difficult concept, is it? And that doesn’t mean heat and cold records aren’t relevant information, too. In fact they are, and there are way more new heat records than new cold records. By 4:1, here is a source for the US.The point is, if there was no change, then there would randomly be new cold and heat records in about the same proportion.

Arctic vs Antarctic: The ice goes away in the north and grows in the south, you say, and accuse scientist of ignoring that. Well, they aren’t, there is lots of research trying to figure out what is happening in both areas. If you think scientist are hiding that ice is growing down south then maybe you are listening to somebody who wants you to think they are hiding it. You can, for instance, look here if you interested in sea ice. For land ice, this might be a good starting point – the question, it says, in under debate.
There are several big differences between the regions that make the simple comparison you demand difficult, to put it mildly. Antarctica is a continent surrounded by ocean and fierce circumpolar currents that basically give it its own independent weather. The Arctic is an ocean surrounded by continents and tightly coupled to what happens with storms, temperatures and precipitation in many countries around it including yours and mine. The northern polar region belongs to a hemisphere that contains two thirds of earth’s land mass – including the major industrial nations with their emission histories. The southern has one third of the land including Antarctica and is otherwise 81 percent water. Why would you expect the two regions to be mirror images? It’s also not as if they were shifting ice masses between them making the changes a zero-sum-game. It is a little like adding the scores of the Chicago Bears, Cubs, Bulls, Black Hawks and Red Stars and comparing them to a sum of clubs from Los Angeles to see which is the better sports town.

And just to finish up with details, your point number 11.

  • Why aren’t insurance companies paying attention to sea level rise? They are. The US flood insurance program is recognized to be in dire need of reform because of rising seas (here, here and here, the last being a report by the National Academy of Sciences). And internationally take the example of Munich Re. It says in its annual report (page 74) that “Climate change represents one of the greatest long-term risks of change for the insurance industry”. It quotes the cost of adapting to sea level rise as 1 trillion dollars for the US alone. And university analysts have given it thumbs up on its response.
  • Why do your beaches look the same? Well, Wikipedia has you living in California, the Eastern Bay area to be exact, and I assume you might be going to Santa Cruz or thereabouts. So let’s look at a map. Here is Santa Cruz with the inundation 2010, 2060 and 2100 (Source). The water has been progressing slowly (and presumably local authorities have taken care of their beaches) but changes will accelerate.
                              Source: Monterey Bay Sea Level Assessment by Noaa

    The numbers in one projection are: seven inches rise last century, six more until 2030, another six until 2050, and two more feet on top by 2100. So you are in the middle of the two-inches-per-decade phase between 2000 und 2030. That’s not easy to notice by naked eye especially when there is maintenance going on. The water will rise, your state authorities say.
  • Why are half the top hits in web searches debunking the rise? Are you honestly asking that? It’s not about the quantity of hits but about the quality of sources. Bloggers have made it their business to attack the science and since they get shared and linked across a parallel universe the Google algorithms think the sites are trustworthy. They get ranked high like science sources who often don’t spend as much time on search engine optimization. For fun try “aliens landing earth” or “Barack Obama muslim” to see the quota of reliable information.

Now for the grand finale, your major and indeed first point: models. Maybe you have been impatiently skipping down here (or maybe you have tuned out) because the IPCC-graph I showed earlier depends on models. How else would you be able to artificially switch off human contributions? That’s nothing we can do in real life.

But why do we need models, in plural, at all? Well, that’s a tenet of science. You make multiple measurements because each one could be flawed, and then you look at what the tendency, the average, the median is, or whatever statistical analysis of them tells you. It’s easy to make fun of that, I know. In everyday life one measurement usually suffices. Has your kid gained another inch, have you lost weight, are the panels of your strip all the right size, is there enough flour in the cookie dough, how many people are at the party, is there enough pressure in your tires, how many e-mails do you get in a single day? Nobody does multiple measurements and statistical analysis for those. Well, for the e-mails you might to eliminate the effect that there could be systematically higher numbers on single days, maybe Mondays and Fridays – just guessing.

In science the questions are usually a whole lot harder, so this multiple measurement habit has proved to be a good idea. As has trusting science as a whole for society, by the way. A consequence of doing this, if you do it right, is that you get two numbers. The result you are looking for and a measure of confidence that that is a figure you can depend on. Scientists being a little nerdy often call the confidence measure “uncertainty”. And that is what it is, too. It is a measure of how uncertain we need to be – or certain we can be. In everyday language that word “uncertainty” comes across badly, just like scientists don’t know their stuff. In fact it is not a weakness but a strength of the scientific method. It has established ways of quality control that are certainly lacking from many other parts of communication – like getting directions in a strange town, remembering the weather two years ago, indicating the size of fish you caught.

When there are no measurement values to be had, because you are talking about a future that hasn’t arrived yet, you need to construct models. They simplify reality in a way that makes it computable, usually leaving out big chunks of the complicated stuff. As the saying goes, they are all wrong, but some are useful. For them also it is a good idea to have several, ideally constructed from scratch in independent, competitive research groups. If they point in different directions, all but one must be getting it wrong. If, however, they point in the same direction that can boost confidence they are getting something right.

Of course they could in principle still all be wrong. Then they all would make the same mistakes and that points to the underlying science being wrong. It’s equations about how pressure and humidity and wind and rain work together and how CO2 and other greenhouse gases influence the radiation that enters and leaves the atmosphere. Complicated stuff but pretty well tested, so the criticism against models is usually not leveled there.

This graph comes from the IPCC. It shows there’s some work to be done but not total chaos. It is a comparison of a whole lot of climate models hindcasting global temperatures over roughly 150 years and through several large volcano eruptions. Black and bold are the observations, red and bold is the average of the models, the thin lines are individual ones, the yellowish shading marks the reference period against which all temperature differences are measured.
Source: AR5, WG1, Figure 9.8, 2013

To prove that the equations and the mechanisms in the models are working together well researchers do this thing called hindcasting that irritates you so. The models start their work at some point in the distant past, fed with the initial conditions from that point in time and then run unguided through the years up to the present. The results can be compared to what actually happened and give the model makers a measurement of how good they are doing. Only if they pass that test can anyone place any confidence in them getting the future right.

„It tells you nothing of their ability to predict the future“, you say. I wonder why you would think you are in the position to judge that. Yes, hindcasting indeed tells us something about the ability to project the future, because the models prove they get the underlying science right. They calculated a future from 1850 onwards based on first principles. It’s just that for us, that future is the past.

I think it is seriously wrong to equate climate models to the financial market. You are missing something rather important. In climate science people actually know what is happening and what causes what. Obviously the minute details of dealings at the Stock Exchange are NOT known even remotely as well. If you insist on that kind of simile: The only thing that would be comparable is if you have a set of publicly available rules and based on that not one financial genius but a whole bunch of them put their predictions of the future into sealed envelopes guarded by a trusted institution only to be opened when the end year envisioned has arrived and all the data are in. And you would only count it as a success if everyone of those guys got it reasonably right.

Anyways, hindcasting of course is not the primary product of the models. It is a necessary quality check, nothing more, nothing less, and should no more be advertised than race track results for a new car model that will never go 150 mph in traffic. The results should be there for the asking, sure. But not center stage. If you see hindcasting as a major part of science communication around climate models you are looking at the communication in a different way from me. It’s safe to assume that neither of us has the right way to look so we both should be careful about what we say about them. I’ll certainly look out for exaggerated hindcasting language in the future. Maybe you can look beyond that.

Remember though I said that models were “ideally constructed from scratch in independent, competitive research groups” – well, not all of them are. There are connections between some research groups but not others. So some researchers are wondering if all the models should be treated equally, they question the model democracy, as they put it. This is brand new stuff. The suggestion is to weight the models and then to do weighted averages. In real life you would be using that technique maybe to locate a sports arena that several clubs will be using – and you want to giving the club with a 100 actives more say than the clubs with 25, 33 or 50 players. So you weight their respective answers with the number of members. In the case of the models the categories that could be used for weighting are independence – the unique efforts get more say – and skill in hindcasting. So there still is not what you wanted, the single best model, but more of a unified voice of them.

Finally to put the whole business into a framing you might understand (and I find weird examples work best to get points across): If you were to hold the secret theory that creating Dilbert was inevitable given your life’s twists and turns – how would you test that? You could recruit a number of young, very young kids and subject about half of them to the same experiences you had to see if they came up with a similar syndicated cartoon series. You would have control groups that get different experiences to test correlates: Could a woman become Scott Adams? Would there be Dilbert if there hadn’t been Little Nemo, Peanuts or Doonesbury before?

And since this in reality would be impossible or inhumane to subject people to you would think of models to give you an answer. Models, in plural, because if you only relied on one you could so easily get it wrong and never even know about it.

If you are seriously interested, it is a good idea to do this in plural. Scientists are seriously interested. And as a citizen following science news you would be better off not pushing for answers that leave out the best science has to offer. And that doesn’t just apply to models, that goes for most of your demands.

Dear Scott Adams,

you ask in your blog post why climate scientists can’t come up with good ways to convince skeptics that climate change is indeed a problem.

Just a screenshot (and a link). All rights lie with Scott Adams.

This might not be the answer you have been waiting for, but it is the answer I need to give. Let me introduce myself: I am a German science journalist, named Chris Schrader. I live in Hamburg. I am only writing this up because in the end of the post you say you’re interested in the psychology, not the science around the climate issue. So am I, that’s why I am bothering with this, but I do also know enough about the science, having worked as a journalist in the field for 15 years. And I am, this is the last of the preliminaries, assuming you have a genuine interest in the question and are, as you say, “on the side of the scientists”. I realize there is some debate about your motives in a related post but let’s go with the benefit of the doubt.

The simple answer to the question in the headline of your post is: There is no way to convince skeptics because they don’t want to be convinced. Indeed their identity and social status often depends on not being convinced. You make it sound as though everybody secretly wanted to believe climate change is real and a serious problem but just needs the right arguments to bring him- or herself to it. Or in a more neutral way of putting it: that everybody weighed the pros and cons in a rational matter and just needed enough good statements by scientists for the scales to come down on the right side. That is a concept the psychology around the issue debunked years ago: the information deficit model. It simply doesn’t work that way. More or better put information will not sway people’s views on contested topics. So if you propose to know  “what it would take to convince skeptics that climate science is a problem that we must fix”, as you say, I seriously beg to differ. This whole discussion is not about what science says or how it says it, it’s only about what people think they want to hear. You yourself seem to making the same point in the last two sentences but draw no conclusions from it.

Saying or thinking that science has to do anything at all before we finally can begin acting on climate change is nothing but a big smoke screen.

Especially on issues like climate change that are political and divisive, people tend to follow the hints in their social groups on what to think. Climate change is the prime example of that, especially in your country. It has become part of the political identity of people what they think about it. You should read up what researchers like Dan Kahan at Yale have to say on it. He does these tests that check peoples’ skills with numbers. When the subject is, say, the efficacy of a skin ointment in removing a rash and the task is comparing outcomes in subjects using it or not, people do okay. It’s a hard problem, but there are reasonable folks coming up with reasonable answers. But change the subject to climate change, or for that matter gun laws, and every evidence for numeracy people have goes out the window. Now they give the answer that falls on their political party’s line no matter the numbers. And the smarter they are and the better with numbers, the bigger the political spread becomes.

This situation is the result of a lot of hard work by some guys at conservative think tanks and other organisations. Read Naomi Oreskes’ book “The Merchants of Doubt” or see the documentary and you’ll get their names. Some of them had been falcons in the cold war, staunch missile-loving anti-communists, who then went on to defend the tobacco industry against the science saying smoking was really bad for you, defend the asbestos industry against science pointing to the risk of lung cancer the fibers posed, and so on: Acid rain, ozone hole, finally climate change, always on the side of industry against science based arguments supporting regulation.

The tactic was in every case an attack on the science and scientists, and spreading doubt was usually enough, hence Oreskes’ book title. They would say: the science is still out, there is no consensus, graphs hint at different explanations – sound familiar? The strategy was to defend the coveted American liberties against overreaching government or what these guys saw as it. They seemed to believe that if the state were allowed to regulate x then it would soon also want to deal with y and z and other letters. x in this instance being carbon emissions, y maybe cars,
z houses, and somewhere down the road gun control, health insurance and other concepts that are toxic and socialist in the view of the American right (and, just as an aside, that even rather conservative states in Europe have no problem with at all).

So these guys certainly didn’t and don’t want to be convinced climate change is a real problem. Maybe they really believe this theory about giving the state not a single ledge to step on, maybe they were and are also paid well by the industries making a profit in an underregulated business. And as long as they have a hold on their share of public opinion through political contacts, religious leaders, talk radio, TV stations like the Fox News network and bloggers they keep selling opposition to the science of climate change as an identity badge for millions of people in the country. So they don’t want to be convinced, either.

That also seriously deflates calling them skeptics. Skepticsm is a virtue in science but there you need enough of a background to voice specific objections and you have to be willing to let yourself be convinced your views are wrong. We could relax the first point to make this not into an elitist issue but the second is fundamental. And we have just seen there is no disposition by that group to be dissuaded of their criticsm.

One of the many signs held up at the February rally in Boston „Stand up for science“. It rightly says that acting on climate can be patriotic. Copyright: C. Schrader

If you take the psychology serious, and there is lots of research showing this, in order to “convince” the “skeptics” you need to embrace social strategies to get people out of the strangleholds they are in. Show them, demonstrate to them how it can be patriotic and christian to be concerned about climate change and that their neighbors, colleagues and friends think that way, too. Get them hooked on Katharine Hayhoe’s Global Weirding series of short videos: She is an evangelical climate scientists with credentials in both supposedly mutually exclusive communities. Even Trump voters don’t want him to dismantle the US climate change policy. They just don’t talk about it, because they think they are a minority. Well, they aren’t and they need to shown by someone they trust. If that’s you: welcome, get to work. Their political conviction is certainly valued. The questions we have to answer need everyone’s ideas, but it should come in at the stage where we discuss the solutions, not whether the problem is real.

Maybe then that only leaves you, Scott Adams: Do you want to be convinced? I mean, there must be some people willing and able to weigh arguments around climate change. Open mind to the question, good common sense, no patience for b/s but respect for clear arguments, willing to admit to change of opinion. It has been sort of the ideal of the western culture since enlightenment. Let’s assume you are, and further assume your questions or demands have anything to do with that. If that is correct, you show yourself as a guy with a no-nonsense-attitude that will not mind coming across gruff. Then presumably you will be able to handle a similar tone. So here goes: I think you are basically misunderstanding and misrepresenting what science is and does and supposedly owes you to a point that makes it hard to presume that all this happens in good faith.

Best, Chris Schrader

For specialists: Some answers to Adams‘ questions/demands.